Academia.eduAcademia.edu
Japhug* Guillaume Jacques rgyalrongskad@gmail.com CNRS-CRLAO-INALCO September 5, 2016 Illustration of the IPA, Journal of the International Phonetic Association * I wish to thank Alexis Michaud and two anonymous reviewers for comments on previous versions of this work; I am responsible for any remaining error. This research was funded by the HimalCo project(ANR-12-CORP-0006) and is related to the Labex EFL (PPC2 Evolutionary approaches to phonology: New goals and new methods (in diachrony and panchrony). Glosses follow the Leipzig glossing rules, except for the following: auto autobenefactive / spontaneous, emph emphatic, genr generic, ifr inferential, inv inverse, lnk linker, testim testimonial. 1 Introduction This paper focuses on the Japhug language (local name /kɯrɯ skɤt/) of Kamnyu village (/kɤmɲɯ/, Chinese Ganmuniao 干木鸟) in Gdong-brgyad area (/ʁdɯrɟɤt/, Chinese Longerjia 龙尔甲), Mbarkhams county (Chinese Maerkang 马尔康), Rngaba prefecture, Sichuan province, China. Japhug belongs to the Sino-Tibetan family, and is one of the four Rgyalrong languages, alongside Tshobdun, Zbu and Situ.1 The description is based on the author’s fieldwork, and the word list and the short story in the appendix have been provided by Tshendzin (Chenzhen 陈珍, female, born 1950), a retired schoolteacher (a native speaker of Japhug, bilingual in Sichuan Mandarin since childhood). Japhug has a highly developed system of ideophones (Jacques 2013b), which present unusual phonological features, in particular rare clusters. In the following discussion, phonemes or clusters found exclusively on ideophones will be treated separately. In addition, about a quarter of the Japhug vocabulary is borrowed from Tibetan, and these loanwords, like the ideophones, fill some gaps in the phonotactic distribution of vowels and consonants (on gap-filling by loanwords see Martinet 1955 [2005], 63-64). These cases are carefully distinguished from the native vocabulary in the analyses that follow, in order to bring out the phonotactics of inherited Japhug vocabulary. Consonants Plosive Affricate Nasal Fricative Approximant Rhotic Lateral unvoiced aspirated voiced prenasalized unvoiced aspirated voiced prenasalized unvoiced voiced sonorant fricative Bilabial Dental/Alveolar /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /n/ /s/ /z/ /m/ /w/ /l/ /ɬ/ Retroflex Alveolo-palatal /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ʂ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /r/ Palatal Velar Uvular /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /ɲ/ /ŋ/ /x/ /ɣ/ /χ/ /ʁ/ /j/ Glottal /h/ In Japhug, syllables follow the template (C) (C) (C)V(C) or (C) (C) (C)V(V) with initial clusters containing at most three consonants, and at most one coda. Given the complexity of possible onsets, it is not practical, in the case of Japhug, to provide an exhaustive list of possible syllables in the language (unlike Naish languages for instance, see Michailovsky & Michaud 2006; Michaud 2012). 2 Simple onsets The consonant inventory of Japhug comprises 50 phonemes as listed in Table 1. There is a general four-way contrast in stops and affricates between unvoiced unaspirated, unvoiced aspirated, voiced and prenasalized. Since monosyllabic words are few, most of the example words provided here involve disyllabic words, whose first syllable illustrates the consonant at issue followed by the vowel /ɯ/, the most common one. For some examples involving possessed nouns (on this topic see Jacques 2014, 6), a possessive prefix must always be present, and we always give here the third person singular prefix /ɯ-/. Table 1: Examples of the consonant phonemes /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /w/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /n/ /s/ /z/ /l/ /ɬ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /r/ /ɯ-pɯ/ /ɯ-pʰɯ/ /babɯ/ /mbɯt/ /tɯmɯ/ /wɯwɯ/ /tɯboʁ/ /tʰɯɣi/ /dɯdɯt/ /ndɯ/ /tɤtsoʁ/ /tsʰɯtʰo/ /dzɯrdzɯr/ /ndzɯpe/ /nɯŋa/ /sɯmat/ /zɯmi/ /lɯlu/ /ɬɯɣnɤɬɯɣ/ /tʂɯmpa/ /tʂʰɯɣ/ /dʐɯɣdʐɯɣ/ /ndʐɯnbu/ /ʂɯŋʂɯŋ/ /rɯ/ ‘its young’ ‘its price’ ‘blackcurrant’ ‘collapse’ ‘sky’ ‘Boletus sp.’ ‘one group’ ‘imp:downstream:come’ ‘turtledove’ ‘appear (rainbow)’ ‘Potentilla anserina’ ‘kid’ ‘straight’ ‘way of sitting’ ‘cow’ ‘fruit’ ‘almost’ ‘cat’ ‘breathing movement’ ‘apron’ ‘maybe’ ‘strong (of tea)’ ‘guest’ ‘clear’ ‘temporary place (nomads)’ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /j/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /ɣ/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /ʁ/ /h/ /ɯ-tɕɯ/ /tɕʰɯwur/ /dʑɯwdʑɯw/ /ndʑɯnɯ/ /ɕɯŋgɯ/ /ʑɯrɯʑɤri/ /cɯ/ /tɤcʰɯ/ /waɟɯ/ /ɲɟɯ/ /ɲɯɣɲɯɣ/ /ɯ-jɯ/ /kɯki/ /kʰɯna/ /gɯgɯɣ/ /ɯ-ŋgɯ/ /ɕaŋɯ/ /xɯrxɯr/ /ɣɯ/ /qɯqli/ /kɯ-sɤqʰɯqʰa/ /mɯɴɢɯ/ /χaŋχaŋ/ /naŋʁɯ/ /hanɯni/ ‘his boy’ ‘blister’ ‘rough’ ‘Angelica sp.’ ‘before’ ‘progressively’ ‘stone’ ‘wedge’ ‘earthquake’ ‘open (it)’ ‘soft and powdery’ ‘its handle’ ‘this’ ‘dog’ ‘very dark (sky)’ ‘inside’ ‘heat (deer)’ ‘round’ ‘genitive’ ‘staring’ ‘naughty’ ‘Ligularia fischeria’ ‘a little orange’ ‘shirt’ ‘a little’ Among the consonants of Japhug, five are only attested in borrowings from Tibetan and/or ideophones: /ɬ/, /ʂ/, /dʐ/, /dʑ/ and /g/. The analysis of prenasalized voiced stops and affricates, of palatal stops and of /ɬ/ as unitary phonemes rather than clusters /NC/, /C+j/ and /l+x/ respectively will be justified in section . The /ɬ/ is slightly aspirated [ɬʰ], unlike other unvoiced fricatives (note that many languages in the area have constrastive aspirated fricatives, see Jacques 2011; Japhug however has no such contrast). As in many languages of the Tibetan area, the /r/ in a trilled retroflex voiced fricative [ɽ͡ʐ] in onset position, sometimes realized as a simple voiced fricative [ʐ]. 3 Consonants clusters Japhug boasts 414 clusters in syllable onset position: 314 clusters with two consonants and 100 with three consonants. Clusters that are only possible at syllable boundaries are not included in this count. Since Japhug is a heavily prefixing language (on which see Jacques 2013a), most noun or verb stems are prefixed, and thus a considerable part of onset clusters are not attested word-initially. For instance, the cluster /zmb/ is only found in the word /tɤzmbɯr/ ‘silt’ which contains a nominal prefix /tɤ-/ (see Table 2). Yet, speakers are able to parse words into syllables; in the case of /tɤzmbɯr/ ‘silt’, the only possible syllabification in /tɤ|zmbɯr/, not */tɤz|mbɯr/, and thus we can ascertain that /zmb/ can be counted as a possible onset in Japhug. On the other hand, in examples like /pjɤnɯndzɯlŋɯz/ ‘he dozed off’, both syllabifications /pjɤ|nɯ|ndzɯl|ŋɯz/ and /pjɤ|nɯ|ndzɯ|lŋɯz/ are possible, so that /lŋ/ is not counted among syllable onset clusters in Japhug. Decisive evidence from partial reduplication A useful test to analyse and classify clusters is partial reduplication, a very productive process which can be applied to both verb and noun stems and has a variety of morphosyntactic functions (see Jacques 2007). When partial reduplication is applied to a syllable, the rhyme of the replicated syllable is changed to /ɯ/ in the replicant. Some clusters are affected by the partial reduplication: when the last consonant of a cluster is one of the non-nasal sonorants (/r/, /l/, /j/, /w/, /ɣ/ or /ʁ/), and the preceding consonant in neither a sonorant nor a sibilant fricative, the sonorant is deleted, as in the derivation /ɲɤ-prɤt/ ‘he cut it’ → /ɲɤ-nɤ-pɯ~prɤt/ ‘he cut it in all directions’.2 When the penultimate consonant of the cluster is a sonorant and the last consonant is a non-nasal sonorant which is not a glide (/r/, /l/, /ɣ/ or /ʁ/), this last consonant is not deleted, as in /ko-wraʁ/ ‘he attached it’ → /ko-nɤ-wrɯ~wraʁ/ ‘he attached it in all directions’. When the prenultimate consonant is a sibilant fricative (/s/, /z/, /ɕ/, /ʑ/) and the last consonant is a non-nasal sonorant which is not a glide (/r/, /l/, /ɣ/ or /ʁ/), there are various possibilities, which are detailed in section . This morphophonological rule is thus crucial in analyzing and classifying consonant clusters. Sonorants that undergo deletion when partial reduplication is applied are henceforth designated as medial consonants, and it is postulated that they do not belong to the same constituent as the rest of the onset. In the following, we present a complete list of consonant clusters in Ja4 phug. Groups only attested in Tibetan loanwords or ideophones (or deideophonic verbs), and not in the native vocabulary, are indicated in lightgray and gray respectively in the tables. We only count clusters in syllable onsets, not clusters only occurring between syllable boundaries, some of which will be treated in section . Clusters not ending in a (non-nasal) sonorant Clusters whose last consonant is not a non-nasal sonorant have a limited number of possible consonants in first position: /w/, /s/, /z/, /ɕ/, /ʑ/, /l/, /r/, /ʂ/, /j/, /ɣ/, /x/, /ʁ/, /χ/, /n/, /m/ and the homorganic nasal, except for a few clusters in stop+/ɕ/. Clusters beginning in /w/ or in a alveolar fricative /s/ /z/ are listed in Table 2. /w/ is realized as [f] or [ɸ] before unvoiced obstruents and as [v] or [β] before voiced ones. /w/ does not appear before nasal or prenasalized segments, and is not compatible with a labial or a uvular segment. Some clusters with /w/ + voiced obstruents (/wz/ and /wg/) are only attested in Tibetan loanwords. Clusters with three consonant whose first element is /w/ and the last one is not a sonorant are all restricted to Tibetan borrowings except /wxt/, which is realized as [xʷt] with a labiovelarized fricative (and labializes preceding /ɯ/ and /ɤ/ to [u] and [o] respectively). Not all speakers maintain the contrast between /wxt/ and /xt/, and the former cluster is only attested in a single word /wxti/ ‘big’. /s/ and /z/ as first element of a cluster are only contrastive before a sonorant. With obstruents, the fricative has the same voicing value as the following consonant. All clusters of this type are attested in the native vocabulary. 5 6 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ Table 2: List of consonant clusters with /w/, /s/ or /z/ as a first element (38+8) /sp/ /ft/ /ɯ-wtaʁ/ sign /wd/ /wdɯt/ demon /wts/ /wtsʰ/ /wtsoʁ/ /wtsʰi/ female hybrid yak it is not serious (disease) /ws/ /wz/ /wsaŋ/ /wzaŋsa/ fumigation friend /wtɕ/ /wtɕʰ/ /wtɕar / /wtɕʰur/ summer he pours it down /wɕ/ /wʑ/ /wtʂ/ /wɕaʁ/ /wʑar / /wtʂi/ he repents for it buzzard he melts it /wc/ /tɯ-wcaʁ / dorsal mat /wɟ/ /wɟi/ he runs after it /spoz/ incense /sm/ /st/ /stʰ/ /smar/ /staχpɯ/ /stʰaw/ river pea he touches it /sn/ /sna/ he is able, worthy /sc/ /scʰ/ /scoʁ/ /scʰɤt/ scoop it comes down (water level) /sɲ/ /sɲaŋne/ fasting /zb/ /zmb/ /zm/ /zbaʁ/ /tɤzmbɯr/ /zmɤrɤw/ dry silt he eat it with /zd/ /znd/ /zn/ /zdɯm/ /znde/ /znɤje/ cloud wall he feels sorry, regrets it /zɟ/ /zɲɟ/ /kɯ-nɯzɟɯ/ /zɲɟa/ suffering losses plant sp. 7 /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /wk/ /wka/ order /wg/ /wgoz/ he prepares it /wxt/ /wst/ /wrt/ /wsk/ /wzg/ /wzd/ /wzɟ/ /wrɟ/ /wxti/ /wstɯn/ /wrtɤn/ /wskɤr/ /wzgɤr/ /wzdɯnɯ/ /wzɟɯr/ /wrɟaŋ/ it is big he serves him he is trustworthy he goes around it he delays it they collect it he transforms it he stretches it (skin) /sk/ /skʰ/ /skɤm/ /rɟɤskʰi/ ox pan /sŋ/ /sŋaʁ/ he curses him /sq/ /sqʰ/ /sqamnɯz/ /sqʰi/ twelve tripod /zg/ /zŋg/ /zga/ /kɤ-ɤkʰɤzŋga/ sauce to call Clusters with /l/, /r/ and /ʂ/ as first element are listed in Table 3. /r/ and /ʂ/ are almost in complementary distribution as first element of a cluster, the former appearing before voiced consonants and the latter after unvoiced ones (except before /ɣ/, see section ). In keeping with this generalization, /r/+nasal clusters are widely attested (nasals are phonemically and phonetically voiced in Japhug), while /ʂ/+nasal clusters are only found in attested in some ideophones. There are some phonotactic constraints on the distribution of these consonants: /l/ is not compatible with coronal fricatives, and /r/ and /ʂ/ never appear before retroflex fricatives and affricates. 8 9 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ Table 3: List of consonant clusters with /l/, /r/ or /ʂ/ as a first element (52+1) /lp/ /tɯ-lpɤɣ/ /lm/ /lt/ /ltʰ/ /ld/ /tɤlmɯz/ /ltɤw/ /ltʰɯmɯmi/ /ldɯɣi/ straw covering the balcony he folds it coming slowly (sleep) bharal /ln/ /lts/ /ltsʰ/ /lni/ /ɕɤltsaʁ/ /ltshɤltshɤt/ it withers leather coat small and weak /ltɕ/ /ltɕʰ/ /ldʑ/ /ʂtɤltɕaʁ/ /ltɕʰɤltɕʰɤt/ /ldʑaŋkɯ/ one piece horse whip hanging (of fluffy objects) green /ldʐ/ /ldʐaŋldʐaŋ/ hanging (big object) /lc/ /lcʰ/ /lcɯɣlcɯɣ/ /tɯ-lcʰɯɣ/ drenching section (of a bag) /rmb/ /rm/ /armbat/ /rmɤwja/ near peacock /rd/ /rnd/ /rn/ /rdɤstaʁ/ /rnde/ /rnaʁ/ stone he finds it it is deep /rdz/ /rndz/ /rdzardza/ /rndzɤkɤŋe/ insolent shade of the mountain /rz/ /tɯ-rzɯɣ/ one section /rndʑ/ /cɯrndʑi/ sand /rʑ/ /tɤ-rʑaw/ wife /rɟ/ /rɲɟ/ /rɲ/ /rɟaʁ/ /rɲɟaʁlo/ /rɲaŋ/ he dances bolt its is ancient /ʂp/ /ʂpʰ/ /tɯ-ʂpa/ /ʂpʰɤwʂpʰɤw/ axe flapping wings /ʂt/ /ʂtʰ/ /ʂtalu/ /ɯ-pɤʂtʰɤw/ horse year middle /ʂts/ /ʂtsʰ/ /ʂtsot/ /ʂtshom/ vengeance it has a crack (bucket) /ʂs/ /ʂsɯwʂsɯw/ hairy /ʂtɕ/ /ʂtɕʰ/ /nɯʂtɕe/ /ʂtɕʰɯʁjɯ/ he teases him caterpillar /ʂɕ/ /rɕɯwrɕɯw/ rough /ʂc/ /ʂcʰ/ /tɤ-ʂcoʁ/ /ɯ-ʂcʰaʂcʰɤw/ mud interstice /ʂɲ/ /ʂɲoʁʂɲoʁ/ long and thin 10 /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /lŋ/ /lx/ /lq/ /lŋɤlŋɤt/ /lxɤwlxɤw/ /lqɤnɤlqɤt/ hanging (fruit) thick (clothes) toddling /rg/ /rŋg/ /rŋ/ /rɴɢ/ /lpɕ/ /qalpɕa/ it opens (fern leaf) /rga/ /rŋgɤm/ /tɯ-rŋa/ /ɕɯrɴɢo/ he likes it hard piece face Anisodus tanguticus /ʂk/ /ʂkʰ/ /ʂko/ /tɤ-ʂkʰom/ it is hard feather rachis /ʂq/ /ʂqʰ/ /ʂqoʁ/ /tɤ-ʂqʰu/ he hugs him bark, skin /ʂχ/ /ʂχɯʂχi/ with big nostrils The glide /j/ and the alveolo-palatal fricatives /ɕ/ and /ʑ/ (Table 4) only occur before labial, dental, velar and uvular stops; they are marginally attested with retroflex affricates. The /j/ glide, unlike other consonants, neither devoices nor fricativizes when occurring as first element of a cluster whose second element is an obstruent. Clusters with /x/, /ɣ/ and /χ/, /ʁ/ as first element are listed in Table 5. These two series of fricatives always share their voicing feature with the following segment when it is an obstruent. With nasal sonorants, they are almost always voiced except in the group /χɲ/, which contrasts with /ʁɲ/ and is only attested in ideophones. The velar fricatives /x/ and /ɣ/ are compatible with all places of articulation except velars and uvulars, but event these are possible in heterosyllabic clusters (see ). 11 Table 4: List of consonant clusters with /j/ or /ɕ/ʑ/ as a first element (29+1) /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /jp/ /jpum/ it is thick /jm/ /jt/ /jmɯt/ /ajtɯ/ he forgets it it accumulates /jn/ /jts/ /jtsʰ/ /jnom/ /tɤ-jtsi/ /jtsʰi/ it is flexible pillar he gives him to drink /jtʂʰ/ /qajtʂʰa/ vulture /jndʐ/ /jndʐɤz/ it is thick (powder) /jk/ /tɤ-jkɯz/ secret /jŋ/ /tɤ-jŋoʁ/ hook /jq/ /jqe/ he is able to lift it /jχ/ /ajχoʁ/ it is flat (belly) /jmŋ/ /tɯ-jmŋo/ dream (n) 12 /ɕp/ /ɕpʰ/ /ɕpaʁ/ /ɕpʰɤt/ he is thirsty he patches it /ʑmb/ /ɕm/ /ɕt/ /ɕtʰ/ /ʑd/ /ʑmbɤr/ /ɕmi/ /ɕte/ /ɕtʰɯz/ /ʑdɯɣʑdɯɣ/ ulcer he mixes it he contaminates him he is turned towards strong, tough /ɕn/ /ɕnat/ weaving implement /ɕtʂ/ /ɕtʂaŋlaŋ/ hanging and swinging /ɕk/ /ɕkʰ/ /ʑg/ /ʑŋg/ /ɕŋ/ /ɕkom/ /ɕkʰo-nɯ/ /ʑgaʁ/ /ʑŋgu/ /ɕŋaʁɕŋaʁ/ muntjac they spread it exactly he crosses river on boat bright yellow /ɕq/ /ɕqʰ/ /ʑɴɢ/ /ɕqɤjɤr/ /ɕqʰaloʁ/ /ʑɴɢɯloʁ/ cross-eyed latch walnut 13 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ Table 5: List of consonant clusters with /x/ɣ/ or /χ/ʁ/ as a first element (49) /xp/ /tɯ-xpa/ /χp/ /χpʰ/ /χpi/ /taχpʰe/ story slap /ɣmb/ /ɣm/ /xt/ /xtʰ/ /ɣd/ /ɣnd/ /ɣn/ /xts/ /xtsʰ/ /tɯ-ɣmba/ /tɯ-ɣmaz/ /xtɯt/ /xtʰom/ /ɣdɤso/ /ɣnde/ /ɣnɤsqi/ /xtsɤɕna/ /xtsʰɯm/ cheek wound wild cat he puts it horizontally species of grub he hits with a hammer twenty tip of boot it is thin /χt/ /χtʰ/ /χtɤrma/ /naχthɤw/ offerings he seizes the opportunity /χts/ /χtsʰ/ /χtso/ /χtsʰɤχtsʰɤt/ it is clean small and active /xs/ /ɣz/ /xsar/ /ɣzɯ/ goral monkey /χs/ /χsɤr/ gold /xtɕ/ /xtɕʰ/ /xtɕi/ /xtɕʰɯt/ it is small it can hold /χtɕ/ /χtɕoŋ/ rheumatism /ɣndʑ/ /xɕ/ /ɣʑ/ /xtʂ/ /ɣndʑɤw/ /xɕaj/ /ɣʑo/ /nɤxtʂi/ fire grass bee he will bring it with him /χɕ/ /χɕu/ it is strong /χtʂ/ /χtʂɯɣdʑa/ butter tea /xʂ/ /xc/ /xcʰ/ /ɣɟ/ /xʂɤxʂɤt / /xcat/ /tɤlɤxcʰi/ /ɣɟaw/ long and thin many curdled milk churn (milk) /χʂ/ /χc/ /χcʰ/ /χʂɤχʂɤt/ /χcoŋkroŋ/ /χcʰa/ light (clothes) cross-legged (sitting) right /ɣɲ/ /ɯ-ɣɲaʁ/ disaster /χɲ/ /χɲɤχɲɤr/ without energy one year /ʁb/ /ʁmb/ /ʁm/ /ʁbɤʁbɤβ/ /aʁmbɯm/ /ʁmaʁ/ thick and big concave army /ʁd/ /ʁnd/ /ʁn/ /ʁdɯɣ/ /ʁndɤr/ /ʁnaʁna/ it is serious it scatters both /ʁndz/ /ʁndzɤr/ he cuts it (with scissors) /ʁz/ /ʁzɤw/ he is careful in /ʁʑ/ /ʁʑɯnɯ/ young man /ʁɟ/ /ʁɲɟ/ /ʁɲ/ /ʁɟa/ /ʁɲɟiʁɲɟi/ /ʁɲɤrpa/ completely enormous steward (monastery) Clusters with nasal segments as first element (not counting voiced prenasalized stops and affricates) are listed in Table 6. We find homorganic nasal clusters, compatible with all places of articulation, and nonhomorganic ones, which can be either clusters in /n/+labial or velar or /m/+non-labial. There are no clusters with a nasal directly followed by a fricative or any non-nasal sonorant. After /n/ and /m/, the contrast between voiced preinitial and voiced consonants is neutralized; the existence of the cluster /mɢ/ while only the prenasalized phoneme /ɴɢ/, not simple voiced /ɢ/ exists, shows that it should be analyzed as /mɴɢ/ phonologically. 14 Table 6: List of consonant clusters with a nasal as first element (38+1) 15 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /mp/ /mpʰ/ /mpɯ/ /mpʰɯl/ it is soft it reproduces /nb/ /nt/ /ntʰ/ /nts/ /ntsʰ/ /ntɕʰ/ /ntaw/ /ntʰɤw/ /ntsɯ/ /ntsʰɤr/ /ntɕʰoz/ /anbaʁ/ he hides it is stable it is caught /mt/ /mtʰ/ /tɤ-mtɯ/ /mtʰɯ/ knot spell always it neighs /md/ /mn/ /mts/ /mtsʰ/ /mda/ /mna/ /tɤ-mtsɯ/ /mtsʰɤm/ it reaches it heals button he hears /mdz/ /mdzadi/ flea /mtɕ/ /mtɕʰ/ /mtɕoʁ/ /tɤ-mtɕʰo/ it is sharp wedge /mdʑ/ /tɯ-mdʑu/ tongue he uses it 16 /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /ntʂ/ /ɲc/ /ɲcʰ/ /ŋk/ /ŋkʰ/ /ntʂu-nɯ/ /ɲcɤr/ /ɲcʰoʁ/ /ŋke/ /ŋkʰor/ /mtʂ/ /kɯ-ɤrɤmtʂɯmtʂaj/ sticky /mdʐ/ /mdʐɯɕɯɣ/ bedbug he presses on it shrinks /mc/ /mcʰ/ /tɤmcar/ /tɯ-mcʰi/ tongs gall he walks he arrives /mɟ/ /mɲ/ /mk/ /mkʰ/ /tɯ-mɟa/ /mɲɤm/ /tɯ-mke/ /mkʰɤz/ jaw species of tree neck he is expert /mg/ /mŋ/ /tɯ-mga/ /mŋɤm/ advantage it hurts /mɢ/ /tɤ-mɢom/ clamp they weed /ng/ /nŋ/ /ɴq/ /ɴqʰ/ /ɴqa/ /ɴqʰi/ it is difficult it is dirty /mpɕ/ /mpɕɤr/ it is beautiful /ngɯt/ /nŋo-nɯ/ it is strong they lose Finally, we find a few clusters comprising a stop followed by the fricative /ɕ/ : /pɕ/ (as in /ɯ-pɕi/ ‘outside’), /kɕ/ (as in /kɯjkɤkɕi/ ‘marten’), /lpɕ/ and /mpɕ/ (see Tables 3 and 6). These clusters historically originate from aspirated stops followed by /j/ (/pʰj/, /kʰj/), but their pattern in reduplication indicates that this is not true any more synchronically. For instance, /mpɕɤr/ ‘it is beautiful’ is reduplicated as /mpɕɯ~mpɕɤr/) not †/mpʰɯ~mpʰʲɤr/) Clusters ending in a (non-nasal) sonorant The non-nasal sonorants (/r/, /l/, /j/, /w/, /ɣ/ or /ʁ/) can occur after any consonant except nasals. In this section, clusters are listed by the penultimate consonant (the one immediately preceding the non-nasal sonorant). The list of all clusters whose final consonant is a glide /j/ or /w/ is provided in Table 7. The glides /j/ or /w/ are medials in all clusters except /wj/, /jw/ /ɣj/ and /ʁj/. The labio-velar /w/ has a very restricted distribution as last element of a cluster; in the native non-ideophonic vocabulary, it only occurs after /l/, /z/ andj and is never found in cluster comprising three consonants. The palatal glide /j/ has a wider distribution: it occurs after all places of articulation except palatal and retroflex. Table 8 provides a list of all clusters whose final consonant is a liquid /r/ or /l/. Clusters ending in /r/ cannot contain another /r/ or /l/ segment, or any retroflex consonant (on the crosslinguistic rarity of the cluster /lr/, see Baroni 2014, 78). Clusters ending /l/ never contain another /l/, but allow the presence of /r/ (/rl/, /rɴɢl/). The sonorants /r/ and /l/ are medials before all stops and affricates, as well as before the voiced fricatives /z/ and /ʑ/. In all other contexts, they are not medials. Table 9 provides a list of all clusters whose final consonant is a dorsal voiced frivative /ɣ/ or /ʁ/. Only one dorsal segment is possible within a onset-initial consonant cluster. The sonorants /ɣ/ and /ʁ/ are medials only before all stops and affricates. Heterosyllabic clusters The list of clusters presented in the previous section only include syllable initial clusters; many more additional clusters are possible across syllable (and morpheme) boundary. Clusters made of the coda of the first syllable and the onset of the second syllable of a disyllabic word that are not attested in simple onsets can be grouped in three categories. First, while in syllable onsets we never find clusters containing two stops, such clusters are found across syllable boundary. In Tibetan loanwords, two clusters with /t/ as first element, /tk/ and /tp/, are attested in words such as /χɕit.ka/ ‘spring’ (Tibetan /dpʲid.ka/) or /rɟɤt.pa/ ‘eighth month’ (Tibetan /brgʲad.pa/). The only other cluster containing two stops, /pt/, is attested in the word 17 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /w/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /l/ /ɬ/ /r/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /j/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /ɣ/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /ʁ/ /h/ Table 7: List of consonant clusters ending in /j/ or /w/ (30+18) /dw/ /dwaŋdwaŋ/ out of his head /zw/ /lw/ /zwɤr/ /lwɤz/ mugwort he will be sick again /rw/ /ʂw/ /rwa/ /aɣɯʂwaŋ/ yak felt tent it comes in pairs /jw/ /kw/ /jwajwa/ /kwitsɯt/ very thin cupboard /xw/ /xwɤrnɤxwɤr/ rotating quickly /χw/ /χwɤr/ Hor (name) /hw/ /hwɤrhwɤr/ wide-mouthed 18 /pj/ /pjalu/ year of the cock /bj/ /mbj/ /bjɯbjɯɣ/ /mbjom/ hanging in great number it is fast /wj/ /tɕʰɯwja/ duck /dj/ /ndj/ /djoʁdjoʁ/ /ndjɤndjɤt/ evenly mixed gracious /tsj/ /tsjaŋnɤtsjaŋ/ very tall, moving /ndzj/ /sj/ /zj/ /lj/ /ndzjaʁ/ /sjaŋnɤsjaŋ/ /zjaŋzjaŋ/ /qaljaʁ/ it is tight (knot) shaking one’s head big eagle /rj/ /tɯ-rju/ word /kj/ /kʰj/ /pa-kjo/ /kʰjɤt/ he caused it to slip gliding /ŋgj/ /ŋgjo/ he slips /ɣj/ /qj/ /qʰj/ /ɴɢj/ /tɯ-ɣjɤn/ /qjaw/ /qʰjɯqʰjɯ/ /ɴɢja/ one time it is bitter blunt (colour) it comes loose /ʁj/ /ʁjit/ he thinks about him /wsj/ /wzj/ /tɤ-wsjit/ /wzjoz/ whistle he learns it /spj/ /spʰj/ /stj/ /sqʰj/ /spjaŋkɯ/ /spʰjar/ /stjaŋnɤstjaŋ/ /sqʰjar/ wolf he dries it jumping he stretches it /ltʰj/ /lbj/ /lthjɤlthjɤt/ /lbjɯlbjɯɣ/ well-ironed (clothes) hanging /ʂpj/ /rmbj/ /ʂtsj/ /ʂqʰj/ /rɴɢj/ /ʂpjɯ/ /tɤ-rmbja/ /ʂtsjaʁ/ /ɯ-ʂqʰjoʁ/ /arɤrɴɢjoʁ/ it is spoiled (milk) flash of lightning it is steep (road) its notch having a notch /χtsj/ /χpj/ /χsj/ /χtsjɯ/ /χpjɤt/ /ɯ-χsjɯw/ pint he observes it its slough /mpj/ /mtsj/ /mpja/ /ɯ-mtsjoʁ/ it is warm its beak /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /w/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /l/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /r/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /j/ /k/ /kʰ/ /g/ /ŋg/ /ŋ/ /x/ /ɣ/ /q/ /qʰ/ /ɴɢ/ /χ/ /ʁ/ Table 8: List of consonant clusters ending in /l/ or /r/ (44+52) /pl/ /plɯt/ he destroys it it is destroyed /pr/ /pʰr/ /br/ /mbr/ /pri/ /kʰɤpʰrɯ/ /brɯbrɯz/ /mbrɤt/ bear spraying water with the mouth having pimples it breaks /mbl/ /mblɯt/ /wl/ /wli/ he plants it /wr/ /wraʁ/ he attaches it /dr/ /ndr/ /droŋdroŋ/ /qɯmndroŋ/ big and dirty wild goose /tsl/ /tslɯɣtslɯɣ/ completely wrapped up /tsr/ /tsri/ it is salty /sl/ /zl/ /sloʁ/ /tɯ-zloʁ/ it digs (with its snout) one time /ndzr/ /sr/ /zr/ /ndzri/ /srɯn/ /zrɯ/ he wrings it cotton sunny side of the mountain /tɕr/ /tɕrɯɣnɤtɕrɯɣ/ crunching /ɕr/ /ʑr/ /ɕri/ /ʑru/ it leaks it is strong /ɕl/ /ɕlu/ he ploughs /rl/ /rlaʁ/ it disappears /cl/ /claŋclaŋ/ round and smooth /cr/ /cʰr/ /ɟr/ /crɯɣcrɯɣ/ /cʰrɤwcʰrɤw/ /ɟrɯɣɟrɯɣ/ in a mess messy and dirty gurgling /jl/ /kl/ /jla/ /klɯklɯɣ/ hybrid yak stiff /gl/ /ŋgl/ /glɤɣglɤɣ/ /cɯŋglɯɣ/ pressed pestle /jr/ /kr/ /kʰr/ /gr/ /ŋgr/ /ɯ-jroʁ/ /krɤɣ/ /kʰro/ /grɯwgrɯw/ /ŋgrɤl/ its furrow he cuts/mows it much matsutake it is usually the case /ɣl/ /ql/ /qʰl/ /ɴɢl/ /ɣle/ /qlɯt/ /qʰlɯ/ /ɴɢlɯt/ he rubs it he breaks it naga it breaks /ɣr/ /qr/ /ɣro/ /qro/ he suffocates pigeon /ɴɢr/ /ɴɢraʁ/ it is torn /ʁl/ /tɯ-ʁla/ forearm /ʁr/ /ʁrɯlu/ without horns 19 /scl/ /sclaŋsclaŋ/ bald /sql/ /sqʰl/ /sqlɯm/ /asqʰlu/ it will sink in it is concave /ɕpl/ /ɕploʁɕploʁ/ round and smooth /ɕkl/ /ɕkliɕkli/ round and stiff /ɕql/ /ɕqʰl/ /ɕqlɯwnɤɕqlɯw/ /ɕqʰlɤt/ walking in the water it disappears /rɴɢl/ /arɴɢlɯm/ it is concave /χpl/ /χploʁχploʁ/ round like a ball /ʁɲɟl/ /ʁɲɟliʁɲɟli/ big and tall /mql/ /mɢl/ /mqlaʁ/ /tɯ-mɢla/ he swallows it one step /wkr/ /wɣr/ /wsr/ /wkrɯz/ /wɣrum/ /wsroŋ/ he is greedy it is white he protects it /spr/ /zbr/ /zmbr/ /stʰr/ /scr/ /zɟr/ /skr/ /skʰr/ /zgr/ /sqr/ /sprɯskɯ/ /zbrilu/ /sɤzmbri/ /stʰrɯβ/ /scraʁscraʁ/ /zɟraŋzɟraŋ/ /skraskra/ /tɯ-skʰrɯ/ /zgrawa/ /sɤsqra/ reincarnated year of the snake he makes him angry dangling (of snot) very small soft and bloated impolite body leather sack limit /ɕpr/ /ʑmbr/ /ɕtr/ /ʑdr/ /ɕkr/ /ʑgr/ /ʑŋgr/ /ɕqr/ /ʑɴɢr/ /aɕprɯm/ /ʑmbri/ /ɕtraŋɕtraŋ/ /ʑdraŋʑdraŋ/ /ɕkrɤz/ /ʑgrɯɣ/ /ʑŋgri/ /ɕqraʁ/ /ʑɴɢro/ it is badly sewed willow long and soft long and soft oak certainly star he is intelligent Jew’s harp /jkr/ /jtsr/ /jkrɯt/ /jtsraw/ it will solidify he delays his departure /xpr/ /ta-ɣɤxpra/ he sent him /χpr/ /ʁmbr/ /χsr/ /ʁzr/ /χcr/ /ʁɟr/ /ʁgr/ /tɕʰɯχpri/ /taʁmbra/ /ɣɤχsrɯ/ /ʁzraŋʁzraŋ/ /χcɯχcri/ /ʁɟɯʁɟri/ /ʁgra/ newt crying and shouting handsome dishevelled thin, diluted fat and soft enemy /ɲcr/ /ŋkʰr/ /ngr/ /ɴqr/ /ɲcɯɲcri/ /ŋkʰrɯli/ /ngrɯβ/ /ɯ-ɴqra/ thin, diluted screw accomplish shabby /mtsr/ /mpʰr/ /mkʰr/ /mgr/ /mɯmtsrɯɣ/ /mpʰrɯmɯ/ /mkʰroŋ/ /mgrɯn/ he drinks it with a straw divination he will be reincarnated he receives him /nbr/ /nbraʁ/ he hoes it 20 /p/ /pʰ/ /b/ /mb/ /m/ /w/ /t/ /tʰ/ /d/ /nd/ /n/ /ts/ /tsʰ/ /dz/ /ndz/ /s/ /z/ /l/ /ɬ/ /tɕ/ /tɕʰ/ /dʑ/ /ndʑ/ /ɕ/ /ʑ/ /tʂ/ /tʂʰ/ /dʐ/ /ndʐ/ /r/ /ʂ/ /c/ /cʰ/ /ɟ/ /ɲɟ/ /ɲ/ /j/ Table 9: List of consonant clusters ending in /ɣ/ or /ʁ/ (32+18) /pɣ/ /pʰɣ/ /pɣa/ /pʰɣo/ bird he flees /mbɣ/ /mbɣaʁ/ it turns over /wɣ/ /tɣ/ /tʰɣ/ /dɣ/ /ndɣ/ /wɣa/ /tɯ-tɣa/ /tʰɣe/ /dɣɤrdɣɤr/ /ndɣɤndɣɤt/ mill one span acorn dumb shaking /tsɣ/ /tsɣi/ it rots /ndzɣ/ /sɣ/ /zɣ/ /lɣ/ /tɯ-ndzɣi/ /sɣa/ /zɣa/ /lɣe/ /tɕɣ/ /tɕʰɣ/ /wʁ/ /wʁa/ he is victorious fang rust maybe it will he digs it /ndzʁ/ /tɯ-ndzʁi/ collar bone /zʁ/ /lʁ/ /zʁɤɲcɯ/ /lʁa/ sling gunny bag /tɕɣaʁ/ /tɕʰɣaʁtɕʰɣaʁ/ he squeezes it out completely /tɕʁ/ /tɕhʁ/ /tɕʁɯznɤtɕʁɯz/ /tɕhʁɯznɤtɕhʁɯz/ crunchy crunchy /ndʑɣ/ /ɕɣ/ /ʑɣ/ /ndʑɣaʁ/ /tɯ-ɕɣa/ /ʑɣɤpa/ it is squeezed out tooth he is arrogant /rɣ/ /ʂɣ/ /tɯ-rɣi/ /ʂɣɤlʂɣɤl/ seed transparent and round /rʁ/ /rʁe/ it puts it through /cʰɣ/ /qacʰɣa/ fox /ɲɟɣ/ /ɲɟɣɤrɲɟɣɤr/ plump and huge /jɣ/ /jɣɤt/ he comes back /jʁ/ /ajʁu/ it is bent /spɣ / /zbɣ/ /stɣ/ /spɣi/ /tɤkɤzbɣaʁ/ /stɣɤrnɤstɣɤr/ storehouse headache jumping /lcʰɣ/ /ldzɣ/ /lcʰɣaʁlcʰɣaʁ / /stoʁldzɣɤm/ nice to wear straw from broad beans /ɕpɣ/ /ɕpʰɣ/ /ɕpɣo/ /ɕpʰɣo-nɯ/ unit of measure they help him escape /jmbɣ/ /jpɣ/ /nɤjmbɣom/ /jpɣom/ he has vertigo it freezes /rmbɣ/ /rpɣ/ /tɤ-rmbɣo/ /ʂpɣo/ drum up on the mountain /ʁmbɣ/ /ʁmbɣi/ sun /mpʰɣ/ /ntɕʰɣ/ /ntʰɣ/ /ntsɣ/ /ntsʰɣ/ /ɲcɣ/ /ɲcʰɣ/ /mpʰɣaʁmpʰɣaʁ/ /ntɕʰɣaʁ/ /antʰɣar/ /ntsɣe/ /nɤntsʰɣɤz/ /ɲcɣɤɲcɣɤt/ /ɲcʰɣaʁ/ very tight it splashes it bounces he sells it he bumps into many people, very noisy birchbark 21 Table 10: Count of consonant clusters type CC CCC total wC; s/zC lC; ʂ/rC jC; ɕ/ʑC x/ɣC; χ/ʁC nC 38 52 29 49 38 Cɕ 2 Cj; Cw Cl; Cr Cɣ; Cʁ 30 44 32 18 53 18 48 97 50 total 314 100 414 8 1 1 0 1 46 53 30 49 39 2 /sqap.tɯɣ/ ‘eleven’. This is the only case of /p/ appearing as coda (instead of /w/) in a non-ideophonic word. Second, although Table 5 shows restricted possibilities for clusters whose first element is a velar or a uvular fricative, in heterosyllabic clusters there are little constraints on the phonotactics of these clusters. The only ones never attested are velar fricatives followed by velar or uvular stops. A velar fricative can be followed by a uvular fricative: the cluster /ɣʁ/ is attested for some speakers between morpheme boundaries in the case of the causative of intransitive verbs with the onset /ʁ/. For instance, the verb /ʁaʁ/ ‘hatch’ has a causative /sɯ-ʁaʁ/ or /sɯɣ-ʁaʁ/ depending on the speaker. Uvular fricatives followed by velar fricatives or stop are also attested, as in /praʁ.kʰaŋ/ ‘cave’. Third, while in homosyllabic clusters nasal elements cannot be followed by fricatives or non-nasal sonorants, as shown in Table 6, such combinations are possible in heterosyllabic clusters, such as in /naŋ.ʁɯ/ ‘shirt’, /to.nɤ.tsɯm.ɣɯt/ ‘he brings it here and there’, /ɲɯ.nɯ.sɯm.ʁɲɯz/ ‘he hesitates’, /kʰoŋ.rɤl/ ‘hollow tree’. The sonority sequencing principle in Japhug Many works in phonology support the idea that all segments of the world’s languages follow a universal scale of sonority (for instance Vennemann 1988; Blevins 1995; Parker 2002; Baroni 2014; see Ohala 1990 for an opposing view). A particularly elaborate version of the sonority hierarchy has been proposed by Parker (2002, 235): (1) low vowels > mid vowels > high vowels>/ə/ > glides > laterals > flaps > trills > nasals > /h/ > voiced fricatives > voiced stops > voiceless fricatives > voiceless stops and affricates 22 The notion of sonority is used in particular to account for observed generalizations in the structure of consonant clusters: in most languages, clusters follow the so-called sonority sequencing principle (SSP Blevins 1995, 210): (2) Between any member of a syllable and the syllable peak, a sonority rise or plateau must occur. According to this hierarchy, in onset clusters, sonorants are expected to be closer the syllable nucleus than obstruents (/prV/ is favoured over /rpV/), and glides to be closer to the nucleus than any other consonant (/ljV/ is favoured over /jlV/). Onset clusters with glides or non-nasal sonorants violating the SSP (/jC/, /wC/, /lC/, /rC/, where C is a consonant lower in the sonority hierarchy) are crosslinguistically rare, but have been documented for instance in Oceanic languages such as Dorig and Hiw (Francois 2010, 405-412). Japhug, like other Gyalrongic languages,3 is rich in SSP-infringing clusters. For instance, no less than seven clusters with a labial consonant and a non-nasal sonorant violating the SSP are attested: /lp/, /jp/, /lm/, /rm/, /jm/, /rmb/. /jmŋ/. More surprisingly, we find clusters violating the SSP without equivalent non-violating clusters. For instance, out of 15 di-consonantal clusters with /j/ as first element (all SSP-infringing except for /jw/), 8 have no equivalent SSP-compliant cluster (/jm/, /jt/, /jn/, /jtsʰ/, /jtʂʰ/, /jndʐ/, /jŋ/, /jχ/). Clusters vs unitary phonemes In section , a list of consonantal phonemes was proposed without justification. In this section, we justify the need to analyze two groups of consonants as unitary phonemes rather than clusters, namely the prenasalized voiced stops, the palatals and /ɬ/. The prenasalized voiced stops and affricates /mb/, /nd/, /ndz/, /ndʑ/, /ndʐ/, /ɲɟ/, /ŋg/ and /ɴɢ/ all have unvoiced and unvoiced aspirated counterparts such as /mp/, /nt/, /nts/, /ntɕ/, /ntʂ/, /ɲc/, /ŋk/ and /ɴq/. Yet, there are two pieces of evidence showing that the prenasalized voiced stops and affricates are of a different nature from the prenasalized unvoiced ones. First, the former can appear in clusters preceded by fricatives or nonnasal sonorants, as in /ʑmbr/, /jndʐ/ or /rɴɢl/, while the latter cannot. Clusters such as */ʑmpr/, */jntʂ/ or */rɴql/ are not tolerated in Japhug. Second, the uvular voiced prenasalized /ɴɢ/ has no simple voiced counterpart */ɢ/, which therefore precludes analyzing /ɴɢ/ as a cluster /n+ɢ/. The palatal stops /c/, /cʰ/, /ɟ/ and /ɲɟ/ is Japhug cannot be analyzed as / velar+/j// clusters, as a clear contrast exists between the palatal series and a velar followed by /j/, in minimal pairs such as /pɯ-ɲɟo/ ‘he had damages’ and /pɯ-ŋgjo/ ‘he slipped’. The differing syllabic structure of the onsets /ɲɟ/ and 23 Table 11: Palatalization contrasts among coronal and dorsals Onset Example Meaning /ts/ /tsj/ /tʂ/ /tɕ/ /c/ /k/ /kj/ /q/ /qj/ /tɤtsoʁ/ /ɯmtsjoʁ/ /tʂoʁ/ /mtɕoʁ/ /co/ /pako/ /pakjo/ /ʂqoʁ/ /qjoʁ/ Potentilla anserina its beak he adds water it is sharp valley he prevailed over him he caused it to slip he hugs him he vomits /ŋgj/ is confirmed by their reduplication patterns: while in the former the palatalization is present on the replicant /pɯ-nɤ-ɲɟɯ~ɲɟo/ ‘he had damages everywhere’, in the latter the /j/ is not replicated as /pɯ-nɤ-ŋgɯ~ŋgjo/ ‘he slipped everywhere’. Japhug presents an impressively high number of palatalization contrasts (see Table 11, where all contrasts are illustrated with the vowel /o/) among coronal and dorsal onsets. The unvoiced lateral /ɬ/ is a marginal phoneme in Japhug, which does not appears in clusters (except heterosyllabic ones, as in /cɯɣɬaj/ ‘symptom whereby the oral cavity becomes white’) and is very rare in the native vocabulary. Yet, its phonemic status is justified by the fact that it contrasts with /lx/; there are no minimal pairs between the two, but the contrast can be indirectly illustrated by examples such as /alxaj/ ‘(his clothes) are not properly put’ /lxɯlxi/ ‘thick and cumbersome’ one the one hand and /pjɤɬɤt/ ‘he became old’ and /ɬɤndʐi/ ‘ghost’ on the other hand. Codas As is common in the Sino-Tibetan family, the inventory of consonants in coda position in Japhug is more restricted than in initial position. Only twelve consonants appear in coda position: /-p/, /-w/, /-m/, /-t/, /-z/, /-n/, /-l/, /-r/, /-j/, /-ɣ/, /-ŋ/, /-ʁ/. The stop /-p/ is only restricted to a few ideophones, and is not found in the inherited non-ideophonic vocabulary and in Tibetan loanwords (except as first element of the heterosyllabic cluster /pt/ in the word /sqap.tɯɣ/ ‘eleven’, as seen in ). The codas /-n/, /-l/ and /-ŋ/ are extremely rare (but not entirely absent) in the non-ideophonic native vocabulary. In word-final position, codas are voiced when followed by a word beginning with a voiced consonant or a vowel, but are devoiced in phrase-final position, before a pause or before a voiceless segment. 24 Table 12: Realization of non-nasal codas in Japhug Coda Example Realization Meaning /w/ /z/ /j/ /l/ /r/ /ɣ/ /ʁ/ /taqaw/ /tɯlaz/ /tɤmuj/ /dal/ /tatar/ /tɤndɤɣ/ /paʁ/ [taqaw] [tɯlas] [tɤmuj]̥ [dal] [tatar̥] [tɤndɤx] [paχ] or [paˁ] needle forehead feather later staff poison pig Table 13: List of vowels in Japhug Vowel Example Meaning /a/ /e/ /i/ /ɤ/ /ɯ/ /y/ /o/ /u/ /qala/ /qale/ /ɟuli/ /lɤpɯɣ/ /rɯlɯ/ /qaɟy/ /tɕɤlo/ /tɤlu/ rabbit wind flute radish little ball fish upstream milk In isolation, word-final /-z/, /-r/, /-j/, /-ɣ/ and /-ʁ/ in particular are realized as [s], [r̥], [j]̥ , [x] and [χ] respectively as in the examples in Table 12. The coda /-ʁ/ can also be realized alternatively as pharyngealization of the preceding vowel. The list of possible combinations between codas and vowels in Japhug is described in . Vowels Japhug has eight vowel phonemes presented in Table 13. The mid-open unrounded vowels /ɤ/ and /e/ are only marginally contrastive: /ɤ/ does not occur in word-final open syllables except in unaccented clitics (like the linker /nɤ/ ‘if’), and /e/ only occurs in the last (accented) syllable of a word. They are only contrastive with the coda /-t/. The vowel /y/ is only found with some speakers in the word ‘fish’ and the verbs derived from it. It is nevertheless contrastive with /ɯ/ and /u/ (as shown by the quasi-minimal pairs /qaɟy/ ‘fish’, /waɟɯ/ ‘earthquake’ and /ɟuli/ ‘flute’). Other speakers pronounce ‘fish’ with a medial /w/ /qaɟwi/. 25 Table 14: List of possible rhymes in Japhug /a/ /e/ /i/ /ɤ/ /ɯ/ /y/ /o/ /u/ /w/ /aw/ /ɤw/ /ɯw/ /p/ /ap/ /m/ /am/ /ɯp/ /ɤm/ /ɯm/ /om/ /t/ /at/ /et/ /it/ /ɤt/ /ɯt/ /yt/ /ot/ /ut/ /n/ /an/ /z/ /az/ /l/ /al/ /r/ /ar/ /j / /aj/ /ɣ/ /ɤt/ /ɯn/ /ɤz/ /ɯz/ /il/ /ɤl/ /ɯl/ /ɤr/ /ɯr/ /ɤj/ /ɯr/ /ɤɣ/ /ɯɣ/ /on/ /oz/ /uz/ /ol/ /or/ /oj/ /uj/ /ŋ/ /aŋ/ /ʁ/ /aʁ/ /ɯŋ/ /oŋ/ /oʁ/ Table 15: Examples of closed syllable rhymes in /-t/ Vowel Rhyme Example Meaning /a/ /e/ /i/ /ɤ/ /ɯ/ /y/ /o/ /u/ /at/ /et/ /it/ /ɤt/ /ɯt/ /yt/ /ot/ /ut/ /tɤtɯsɤlat/ /tɤtɯnɤmɤlet/ /tɤtɯrɤlit/ /jɤtɯlɤt/ /tʰɯtɯplɯt/ /lotɯznɯqaɟyt/ /nɯtɯsɤwlot/ /pɯtɯnɤlut/ you you you you you you you you boiled it did it reimbursed it threw it destroyed it let him fish took care of him milked it ɯ• u i• y ɤ• o e• a• Rhymes There are strong phonotactic constraints on possible rhymes in Japhug. The only coda attested with all vowels is /-t/ (see Tables 14 and 15); the rhymes /-et/, /-yt/ are only attested in the perfective second singular forms (which have a suffix /-t/ in the variety of Japhug under study). Before /-j/, the contrasts between /ɯ/ and /i/ on the one hand, and /ɤ/ and /e/ on the other hand, are neutralized. The rhyme /-aj/ is realized as [ɛj]. In closed syllables with an alveolo-palatal or a palatal consonant preceding the vowel, the vowel /ɯ/ is fronted and its contrast with /i/ is neutralized in nearly all positions. It is only maintained before /-t/ in forms with the past /-t/ transitive suffix. For instance, we find the minimal pair /tɤ-tɯ-cɯ-t/ ‘you opened it’ (pfv-2-open-pst) and /lɤ-tɯ-cit/ ‘you moved’ (pfv-2-move). 26 Table 16: Examples of associative adverbs in Japhug Base noun Meaning Associative Meaning /tɤ-jwaʁ/ /χɕɤlmɯɣ/ /kɤ́-jwɯ~jwaʁ/ /kɤ́-χɕɤlmɯ~lmɯɣ/ leaf glasses with its leaves with his glasses Suprasegmentals Unlike other Rgyalrong languages (Sun 2005, Lin 2012), Japhug has no tonal contrasts. However, there is morphologically determined stress. Phonological words only have one stress, which is located by default on the final syllable of the word (regardless of its part of speech). The personal agreement suffixes and the evidential suffix /-ci/ never receive stress, and their vowels are optionally devoiced. For instance, /tɤ-ndza-t-a/ ‘I ate it’ (pfv-eat-pst-1sg) is realized [tɤndzátḁ] or [tɤndzáta]. In verbal forms with these prefixes, stress is penultimate, or even antepenultimate in the case of verb forms with two suffixes as in /to-k-ɤmɯ-rpú-ndʑɯ-ci/ ‘they bumped into each other’ (ifr-evd-recip-bump-du-evd). Only two verbal prefixes, the inverse /-wɣ-/ and the negative testimonial /mɯ́ j-/, attract stress, as in /pɯ-tɯ́ -wɣ-mto/ ‘he saw you’ and /mɯ́ j-ndze/ ‘he does not eat it’. The only other morphological process in the language that influences stress is the comitative adverbs formation. Theses adverbs meaning ‘together with X’ are built by combining the stress-bearing /kɤ́-/ prefix with a reduplicated noun stem, as in the examples in Table 16. Appended text This text was translated into Japhug from the Chinese version of Aesop’s fables by Chenzhen 陈珍 /tsʰɯndzɯn/ on May 2014. (3) qale cʰo tɤŋe kɤ-ti ɲɯ-ŋu. kɯɕɯŋgɯ tɕe, wind comit sun inf-say testim-be long.ago lnk iɕqʰa, akɯcʰoʁle cʰondɤre tɤŋe ni kɯ, nɤkinɯ, the.aforementioned northern.wind comit ɲɤ-sɤfɕɤra-ndʑɯ ɲɯ-ŋu sun du erg dem ifr-argue-du testim-be The sun and the wind. Long ago, the north wind and the sun were arguing, 27 (4) ɲɤ-sɤfɕɤra-ndʑɯ tɕe, “tɕɯʑo ɕɯ kɯ-fse ifr-argue-du lnk 1du who nmlz:S/A-be.like kɯ-χɕu me-tɕɯ?” to-ti-ndʑɯ, nmlz:S/A-be.strong not.exist-1du ifr-say-du They argued, they said “Who is the strongest of us?” (5) lɯski tɕe tɕe tɤŋe nɯ kɯ “aʑo χɕu-a” ɲɤ-sɯso, of.course lnk lnk sun dem erg 1sg be.strong-1sg ifr-think qale nɯ kɯ “aʑo χɕu-a” ɲɤ-sɯso tɕe, tɕeri, nɤkinɯ, wind dem erg 1sg be.strong-1sg ifr-think lnk but dem maka ʑo kɤ-sɤfɕara kɤ-sɤpe mɯ-pjɤ-cʰa-ndʑɯ, at.all emph inf-discuss inf-make.better neg-ifr.ipfv-can-du kɯ-maqʰu tɕe, to-nɯkrɤz-ndʑɯ tɕe, tɕe nɤki, nmlz:S/A-be.after lnk ifr-discuss-du lnk lnk dem.prox Of curse, the sun thought “I am strong”, the wind thought “I am strong” and they could not settle the argument. Finally, they decided, (6) “ɕɯ kɯ tɯrme, nɯ kɯ-nɤŋkɯŋke tɯrme ra who erg man dem nmlz:S/A-walk.around man pl nɯ-ŋga ɲɯ-kɤ-sɯ-tɕɤt kɯ-cʰa nɯnɯ, 3pl.poss-clothes ipfv-inf-caus-take.off nmlz:S/A-can dem ɕɯ pɯ-kɯ-wʁa a-pɯ-ŋu” to-nɯ-pa-ndʑɯ. who pfv-nmlz:S/A-prevail irr-pst.ipfv-be ifr-auto-do-du “Whoever can cause the people walking around to take off their clothes will be the victor”, they agreed. (7) tɕendɤre tɯrme tɯ-rdoʁ jo-ɣi tɕe, tɕendɤre qale nɯ, lnk man one-piece ifr-come lnk lnk wind dem akɯcʰoʁle ntsɯ to-wzu tɕe, northern.wind always ifr-make lnk Then, a man came and the wind, the northern wind blew. (8) nɯnɯ iɕqʰa, to-wzu nɤ to-wzu tɕendɤre dem the.aforementioned ifr-make lnk ifr-make lnk iɕqʰa nɯ, ri tʂu kɯ-ŋke tɯrme nɯ the.aforementioned dem but path nmlz:S/A-walk man kɯ nɯ-ŋga ra ko-sɯ-ɤsɯɣ-nɯ ʑo tɕe, ra dem pl erg 3pl.poss-clothes pl ifr-caus-be.tight-pl emph lnk It blew and blew, and the people walking on the road wore their clothes tighter, 28 (9) tɕendɤre, iɕqʰa nɯ, qale kɯ nɯ pa-mto lnk the.aforementioned dem wind erg dem pfv.3>3-see tɕendɤre mɤʑɯ ʑo kɯ-wxti to-wzu. lnk more emph nmlz:S/A-be.big ifr-make The wind saw that, and blew even harder. (10) tɕendɤre, kɯ-nɤŋkɯŋke nɯ ra tɤndʐo kɯ nmlz:S/A-walk.around dem pl cold lnk ɲɤ-sɤ-ndzɯrndzɯr ʑo ifr-deideoph:caus-shivering emph lnk mɤʑɯ ʑo kɯ-dɤn erg tɕendɤre tɯ-ŋga genr.poss-clothes to-ŋga-nɯ pjɤ-ra. more emph nmlz:S/A-be.many ifr-wear-pl ifr.ipfv-need Then, it made the people who were walking shiver from the cold, and they wore even more clothes. (11) toʁde tɕe tɕendɤre to-wzu an.instant lnk lnk ɲɤ-ɲat. ɲɤ-ɲat nɤ to-wzu tɕe tɕendɤre, “wo, aʑo nɯ oh ifr-be.tired ifr-be.tired lnk lnk mɯ́ j-cʰa-a”, ri qale ifr-make lnk ifr-make loc wind tɤŋe ɯ-ɕki, ma 1sg dem apart.from “nɤʑo wra tɤ-tsʰɤt” neg.testim-can-1sg sun 3sg.poss-dat 2sg turn imp-try to-ti. ifr-say After a moment, the wind became tired as he blew without pause. He told the sun: “I can’t do it any more, it is your turn, try it!” (12) tɕe tɤŋe nɯ kɯ kɯ-mɤku tɕendɤre, lnk sun dem erg nmlz:S/A-be.after lnk kɯ-sɤɕkɯ~ɕke mɯ-jo-tɕɤt kɯ, kɯ-mɤku nmlz:S/A-emph~be.hot neg-ifr-take.off erg nmlz:S/A-be.after tɕendɤre ɯ-ɣot lnk jo-ɕtʰɯz, nɯ kɯ-ndʑɯ~ndʑɤm ʑo 3sg.poss-sunray dem nmlz:S/A-emph~be.warm emph tɕe tɯrme ra nɯ-ɕki ifr-turn.towards lnk man jo-ɕtʰɯz. pl 3pl.poss-dat ifr-turn.towards In the beginning, the sun did not send hot (sunrays), he sent warm sun rays on the people, (13) tɕendɤre tɯrme ra nɯnɯ nɯ-mpja-nɯ lnk kɯ-mɤku man jamar tɕe, pl dem auto-be.warm-pl about lnk nɯ-ŋga tɤ-kɤ-ɣɤjɯ nɯ ra nmlz:S/A-be.after 3pl.poss-clothes pfv-nmlz:P-add dem pl ɲɤ-tɕɤt-nɯ, ifr-take.off-pl 29 Then the people, as they felt warm, first took off the clothes that they had added. (14) tɕeri nɯ ɯ-mpʰru tɕe tɤŋe kɯ li, mɤʑɯ ʑo but dem 3sg.poss-following lnk sun erg again more emph kɯ-mpja nɯnɯ, sɤtɕʰa ɯ-taʁ earth 3sg.poss-on nmlz:S/A-be.warm dem pjɤ-ɕtʰɯz, pjɤ-lɤt, ifr:down-turn.towards ifr:down-throw But then, the sun sent even warmer (rays) on the earth, (15) tɕendɤre tɯrme ra kɯ, tɕendɤre nɯ-ɕtʂi man lnk rɯβnɤrɯβ ʑo ʑo nɯ 3pl.poss-sweat emph dem pl erg lnk pjɤ-ɬoʁ. flowing emph ifr:down-come.out The people started to be drenched in sweat. (16) tɕe nɯ ʑɯrɯʑɤri tɕe, nɯ-ŋga ra lonba ʑo lnk dem progressively lnk 3pl.poss-clothes pl all emph ɲɤ-tɕɤt-nɯ ifr-take.off-pl Progressively, they took off all they clothes, (17) tɕe, iɕqʰa nɯ, iɕqʰa... lnk the.aforementioned dem the.aforementioned tɯ-ci ɯ-ŋgɯ tɕe kɯ-raχtɕɯʁɟo indef.poss-water 3sg.poss-inside lnk nmlz:S/A-bathe pjɤ-ɕe-nɯ pjɤ-ra. ifr:down-go-pl ifr.ipfv-need and they went to the river to bathe. (18) tɕendɤre kɯki lnk maŋe χpi ki nɯ ma dem.prox story dem.prox dem apart.from ri, kɯki pɯ-pɯ-ŋu nɤ, not.exist:testim loc dem.prox cond-pst.ipfv-be lnk This story is just that. (the moral of this story is) (19) nɤkinɯ, iɕqʰa dem kɯ-fse, nɯ, daltsɯtsa ɲɯ-kɤ-nɯkʰɤda the.aforementioned dem slowly tu-kɤ-ti nmlz:S/A-be.like ipfv-inf-say lnk force ɯ-mbrɤzɯ smɯn ipfv-inf-persuade tɕe, tɤrkopa ɣɯ́ -wzu kɤ-ti ɲɯ-ŋu. sɤz ndɤre, inv-make comp lnk 3sg.poss-result be.ripe inf-say testim-be persuading people slowly gives better result than forcing them, 30 (20) nɯ kɯ pʰɤn, ɯ-pʰɤntʰoʁ tu kɤ-ti ɲɯ-ŋu. dem erg be.efficient 3sg.poss-advantage exist inf-say testim-be it is more efficient, more advantageous, it is said. Notes See Sun (2000a) for an overview of the Rgyalrong group, whose closest relatives include Khroskyabs (Lai 2015) and Horpa (Sun 2007). A text collection of Japhug with sound files is included in the Pangloss archive (Michailovsky et al. 2014). A short grammar (Jacques 2008), a series of articles on morphosyntax (see for instance Jacques 2013a and Jacques 2014) and a dictionary (Jacques 2015) are available but little has been published specifically on its phonology. 2 The morphological process illustrated here is the non-directed motion derivation, combining a derivational /nɤ-/ prefix with partial reduplication of the verb stem. 3 See for instance Sun (2000b) and Lai (2013, 25-29)). 1 References Baroni, Antonio. 2014. The Invariant in Phonology. The role of salience and predictability: Università degli Studi di Padova dissertation. Blevins, Juliette. 1995. The syllable in phonological theory. In John A. Goldsmith (ed.), Oxford: Blackwell, 206–244. 199. Francois, Alexandre. 2010. Phonotactics and the prestopped velar lateral of Hiw: resolving the ambiguity of a complex segment. Phonology 27(3). 393–434. Jacques, Guillaume. 2007. La réduplication partielle en japhug, révélatrice des structures syllabiques. Faits de langues 29. 9–21. Jacques, Guillaume. 2008. 嘉絨語研究 Jiāróngyǔ yánjiū (Study on the Rgyalrong language). Beijing: Minzu chubanshe. Jacques, Guillaume. 2011. A panchronic study of aspirated fricatives, with new evidence from Pumi. Lingua 121(9). 1518–1538. Jacques, Guillaume. 2013a. Harmonization and disharmonization of affix ordering and basic word order. Linguistic Typology 17(2). 187–217. Jacques, Guillaume. 2013b. Ideophones in Japhug Rgyalrong. Anthropological Linguistics 55(3). 256–287. 31 Jacques, Guillaume. 2014. Denominal affixes as sources of antipassive markers in Japhug Rgyalrong. Lingua 138. 1–22. Jacques, Guillaume. 2015. Dictionnaire Japhug-Chinois-Français, version 1.0. Paris: Projet HimalCo. http://himalco.huma-num.fr/. Lai, Yunfan. 2013. La morphologie affixale du lavrung wobzi. Université Paris III MA thesis. Lai, Yunfan. 2015. The person agreement system of Wobzi Lavrung (Rgyalrongic, Tibeto-Burman). Transactions of the Philological Society 113(3). 271–285. Lin, Youjing. 2012. By no means marginal: privative tone in Zhuokeji rGyalrong. Language and Linguistics 13.4. 625–662. Martinet, André. 1955 [2005]. Économie des changements phonétiques. Paris: Maisonneuve et Larose. Michailovsky, Boyd, Martine Mazaudon, Alexis Michaud, Séverine Guillaume, Alexandre François & Evangelia Adamou. 2014. Documenting and researching endangered languages: the Pangloss Collection. Language Documentation and Conservation 8. 119–135. Michailovsky, Boyd & Alexis Michaud. 2006. Syllabic inventory of a Western Naxi dialect, and correspondence with Joseph F. Rock’s transcriptions. Cahiers de Linguistique - Asie Orientale 35:1.. 3–21. Michaud, Alexis. 2012. The Phonology of Laze: Phonemic Analysis, Syllabic Inventory, and a Short Word List. Yuyanxue Luncong 45. 196–230. Ohala, John J. 1990. Alternatives to the sonority hierarchy for explaining segmental sequential constraints. Chicago Linguistic Society 26(2). 319– 338. Parker, Steve. 2002. Quantifying the sonority hierarchy: University of Massachusetts, Amherst dissertation. Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2000a. Parallelisms in the Verb Morphology of Sidaba rGyalrong and Lavrung in rGyalrongic. Language and Linguistics 1(1). 161–190. Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2000b. Stem Alternations in Puxi Verb Inflection: Toward Validating the rGyalrongic Subgroup in Qiangic. Language and Linguistics 1(2). 211–232. Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2005. Jiāróngyǔzǔ yǔyán de yīn’gāo: liǎnggè gè’àn yánjiū 嘉戎語組語言的音高:兩個個案研究 (On Pitch in the rGyalrongic Languages: Two Case Studies). Yuyan yanjiu 語言研究 25(1). 50–59. 32 Sun, Jackson T.-S. 2007. Morphological causative formation in Shangzhai Horpa. Bulletin of Chinese Linguistics 1(2). 207–227. Vennemann, Theo. 1988. Preference laws for syllable structure and the explanation of sound change : with special reference to German, Germanic, Italian, and Latin. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 33